What is it about?

Over the past decade much has been written about how children vary in their susceptibility to environmental influences and thus developmental plasticity, with some being highly susceptible or plastic and others not so. "Orchid" has been used to label those highly susceptible to both positive and negative exposures and experiences and "dandelion" for those not shaped or influenced very much, if at all, by the same exposures and experiences. So the question becomes is susceptibility a general trait or is it the case that one child might be highly susceptible to some experiences but highly unsusceptible to others? We addressed this issue by studying susceptibiltiy to parent and peer effects influencing adolescent well-being. We discovered that while there was a general--even if limited--trend for children most--or least--susceptible to one source of influence to be similarly susceptible to the other, this average tendency masked an important reality: there is a not insubstantial number of children who prove HIGHLY susceptible to the effects of parenting, but HIGHLY UN-susceptible to the effects of peers, with the same being true of the reverse. This challenges the notion of there being a general trait of susceptibility or developmental plasticity, while raising interesting issues about interventions to promote general well-being: Perhaps different children who are differentially susceptible to different sources of influence--parents rather than peers or peers rather than parents--should be treated in different ways, in this case some via a parenting intervention and others via a peer intervention.

Featured Image

Why is it important?

This is important because, as noted, it challenges the view that variation in susceptibility to environmental influences--with some children being highly susceptible and others not so--is a general trait. To a not insubstantial extent, it raises the real possibility that each child may be a mosaic of susceptibility, thus being highly susceptible to some exposures and not so much to others. To the extent this is generally true, it means efforts to influence children's development may need to be related to the exposures to which children are most susceptible. In other words, just as in personalized medicine, psychosocial interventions may need to be tailored to the characteristics of the particular child. What remains to be determined, of course, is why, for example, some children are more responsive to parental influence and others to peer influence--and still others to both and others to neither.

Perspectives

I am one of the main theorists who have argued, based on evolutionary reasoning and the research it stimulated, that children very or not very much affected by adverse experiences are similarly affected by positive experiences. That is, it is not so much that some children are "vulnerable" to and others "resilient" in the face of adversity, but that some are highly influenced by their experiences and others not so. But I have always wondered if this was a general trait or it varied within the same child depending on the exposures under consideration--in the current example, across parental and peer influence. The fact that a not insubstantial children appeared highly susceptible to one of these sources of influence and highly unsusceptible to the other challenges the general-trait assumption that some children are so-called orchids and others dandelions.

Jay Belsky

Read the Original

This page is a summary of: Beyond early years versus adolescence: The interactive effect of adversity in both periods on life-course development., Developmental Psychology, November 2021, American Psychological Association (APA),
DOI: 10.1037/dev0001247.
You can read the full text:

Read

Contributors

The following have contributed to this page